2.3. Radical morphological processes.
2.3.0.1. The radical morphological processes include all
morphology that is not prefixation or suffixation.
Radical morphological
processes may also be called non-concatenative morphology since they involve
morphemes that are not merely attached to the ends of the stem but are internal
alterations or copies of it.
The radical morphological processes active in Saanich are reduplication,
infixation, ablaut, stress shift/metathesis, and glottalization.
These processes are felicitously grouped together in Saanich for
several reasons. First, they all seem to indicate an allied set of notions:
diminutive, collective, distributive, repetitive, continuative, resultive,
etc. Taking Comrie’s (1976) definition of aspect referring to "the internal
temporal constituency of a situation," and the fact that all Saanich stems are
basically verb-like, we can see that all of the radical morphological processes
mark various aspects in a broad sense.
Second, they are formally allied in opposition to the concatenative
processes of suffixation and prefixation in that all morphemes involving
processes of infixation, ablaut, and stress shift/metathesis have
reduplicative allomorphs, while neither prefixes nor suffixes do. In fact,
morphemes that involve prefixation and suffixation have only prefixal or
suffixal allomorphs.
Of the seven reduplicative patterns two (§2.3.1 and
§2.3.2) occur with only reduplicative allomorphs. Moreover, when
these processes cooccur there arise some interesting complications that appear
with no other morphological process.
A strict linear ordering of application of these processes
can be established
(§2.3.8).
The third and least significant reason for grouping these processes
together is convenience. Because of their radical altering nature and their
formal interaction, these processes present the most difficulties to anyone
attempting to parse a Saanich word. The task will be made easier by grouping
them together.
2.3.0.2. Six reduplicative C-V patterns have been
observed in Saanich:
C1V́C2+, C1C2+, C1V́+, C1+, +C1í, and +C2. All C-V
notations refer to the consonants and vowels of the root only. Reduplicative
processes in Saanich never affect prefixes or suffixes.
Reduplication in Saanich seems to function as a
morphological process in
itself distinct from affixation. Besides the patterning with the other radical
morphological processes noted above, reduplication differs from affixation in
two other ways. First, no prefixes in Saanich can carry stress, but three
regressive, "prefixal", reduplicative patterns are stressed (§2.3.1,
§2.3.5.1, and §2.3.5.5). And, second, while infixation ignores prefixes it does
not ignore reduplication. That is, infixes can be inserted into prefixal
reduplicative material (see §2.3.3.4.3).
There are two infixes in Saanich:
/(ʔ)/ ‘actual’ (§2.3.5) and /(l)/ ‘plural’ (§2.3.3). Each of
these has a reduplicative allomorph, and ‘actual’ has in addition a stress
shifted or metathesized allomorph. The ‘actual’ aspect is also always
accompanied by the glottalization of all non-initial resonants in the word.
Ablaut of the stressed root vowel, ə/e or
ə/a, is the primary way the ‘resultive’ (§2.3.6) is marked.
The ‘resultive’ also has a reduplicative allomorph.
Substitution of /e/ for
an underlying /ə́/ accompanies the ‘actual’ infix (§2.3.5).
2.3.1. /C1V́C2+/ ‘characteristic’1.
This pattern occurs
without significant variation and seems to be fairly productive. Words with
this morpheme are descriptive predicates emphasizing a characteristic trait,
tendency, or disposition. A number of animal names involve this pattern.
When primary stress is attracted by certain strong suffixes the stress
on the vowel will be reduced. The vowel of the root is
reduced to unstressed [ə] and is subject to deletion (see §1.5.4,
§1.5.6).
I have analyzed this as regressive (prefixal) reduplication. An
alternative would be to consider it progressive (suffixal with two consonant
roots and infixal with roots having more than two consonants). There does seem
to be some evidence that the regressive analysis is better. The evidence comes
from words exhibiting complex reduplication, that is, the cooccurrence of the
‘characteristic’ pattern with another reduplicative morpheme.
1. séy̕+√siʔ sən ‘I’m afraid.’
2. x̣íʔ+√x̣əʔ sən ‘I’m embarrassed.’
Examples 1 and 2 are typical cases of the ‘characteristic’
reduplication pattern. When combined with the C1+ ‘resultive’ reduplication
(§2.3.5.6) examples 3 and 4 are the products.
3. séy̕+sə+√siʔ sən
‘I’m scared.’
4. x̣íʔ+x̣ə+√x̣əʔ sən
‘I’m embarrassed.’
If the ‘characteristic’ morpheme were a case of progressive
reduplication, we could expect *x̣ə+√x̣íʔ+x̣əʔ and *sə+√séy̕+siʔ since the
‘resultive’ reduplication is clearly regressive.
The ‘characteristic’ morpheme
must precede2 the ‘resultive’ morpheme.
5. s-néw̕+√nəw̕ kʷə ‘They’re in now.’ (This form
also shows the ‘resultive’ (§2.3.6) of √nəw̕ ‘be inside’.)
6. qʷə́l+√qʷəl ‘He’s talkative.’ (√qʷəl ‘speak’)
7. čéy+√či ‘He’s diligent.’
(√čey ‘work’)
8. mə́t̕+√mət̕ ‘It’s flexible.’
(√mət̕ ‘bend’)
9. q̕ə̀m̕+√q̕əm̕=éčən ‘A
type of wasp.’ (√q̕əm̕
‘cut in two’)
10. x̣éʔ+√x̣əʔ-ɬ=net ‘Sunday.’ (√x̣eʔ ‘sacred’)
11. ƛ̕ə́x̣ʷ+√ƛ̕əx̣ʷ ‘Oyster.’ (√ƛ̕əx̣ʷ ‘hard’)
12. s-t̕θéʔ+√t̕θəʔ ‘He’s mounted (a horse).’
(√t̕θeʔ ‘upon, high’)
13. š-qén̕+√qən̕-s ‘He’s a thief.’ (√qen̕
‘steal’)
14. t̕θə́x̣+√t̕θəx̣ ‘Stinging nettles.’ (√t̕θəx̣
‘poison’)
Some words have been recorded that certainly involve this
morpheme, though their roots do not seem to occur independently.
15. séxʷ+√səxʷ ‘He’s lazy.’
16. sə́l̕+√səl̕qʷ ‘He’s lonesome.’
17. q̕ʷə́l+√q̕ʷəl ‘It’s tame.’
(√q̕ʷəl ‘cooked,
ripe’)
18. t̕ə̀l+√t̕əl-él̕s-əŋ ‘It’s numb.’
19. čə́l̕+√čəl̕xʷ ‘It’s loose.’
20. k̕ʷám+√k̕ʷəm ‘He’s strong.’
21. šə́n̕+√šən̕ ‘He’s alone.’
22. s-t̕ə́l+√t̕ələq ‘It’s speckled.’
23. k̕ʷə́y̕+√k̕ʷiʔ ‘He’s hungry.’
A few words whose roots do not occur elsewhere
seem to bear this reduplicative pattern,
but connections between the meanings of
the words and the meaning of the morpheme ‘characteristic’ are obscure:
24. s-q̕ə́ɬ+√q̕əɬ ‘Muskrat.’
25. s-kʷə́n̕+√kʷən̕ ‘Carbuncle.’
26. s-x̣ʷə́y̕+√x̣ʷiʔ ‘Masked dancer.’
27. sə́l̕+√səl̕-tən ‘Leg spindle.’
28. mús+√məs ‘Cow.’ (< Chinook Jargon)
29. s-q̕ə̀m+√q̕əm-íl ‘High tide.’
30. ŋél̕+√ŋəl̕ ‘Fish bait.’
31. s-x̣ə́m̕+√x̣əm̕ ‘Horsetail.’
(√x̣əm̕ ‘heavy’?)
32. t̕θíxʷ+√t̕θəxʷ ‘Fish hawk.’
(√t̕θixʷ
‘pity’?)
2.3.2. C1+...(ʔ) ‘diminutive.’ This morpheme is,
unfortunately, poorly represented in the corpus. There are enough examples,
however, to discern some patterns.
The reference of stems with this pattern is always to something less in
size, number, or frequency than the reference of the stem without it. This
morpheme is distinct from one meaning ‘young of a species’ (see example 49
below and §2.2.10.1.18).
2.3.2.1. As the notation above implies, the basic form of
this morpheme is discontinuous: reduplication of the first root consonant is
accompanied by a glottal stop infixed after the stressed vowel (see
§1.5.8 on epenthetic schwa):
33. pə+√pá(ʔ)xʷ-ən ‘Small sail.’ (páxʷən ‘sail’)
34. s-tə+√tá(ʔ)kən ‘Little sock.’ (stákən ‘sock’ < English)
35. s-čə+√čə́(ʔ)s=əqʷ ‘Little hat.’ (sčə́səqʷ
‘hat’)
36. kʷə+√kʷí(ʔ)θ=nəč ‘It’s tilted a little.’
(kʷəθnéčt ‘tilt it’)
37. ʔiʔ kʷə+√ké(ʔ)yəl ‘It’s dawn.’ (kʷéčəl
‘It’s daytime.’ See §1.5.9 on y ∼ č.)
38. k̕ʷə+√k̕ʷá(ʔ)
təʔ ‘Crow.’ (sk̕ʷtáʔ ‘raven’)
2.3.2.2. When the consonant following the stressed vowel
is a resonant or glottal stop, the infix coalesces with it. This differs from
the ‘actual’ infix, which does not merge with a following /ʔ/.
See §2.3.5.1.
39. č̕ə+√č̕ə́y̕əƛ̕ ‘He’s short.’
40. k̕ʷə+√k̕ʷə́w̕yəkʷ ‘Small fish
hook.’
(k̕ʷə́wyəkʷ ‘fishhook’)
41. s-qə+√qx̣=áʔ=aɬ ‘Little puppy.’ (sqéx̣əʔ
‘dog’)
42. mə+√mím̕ən ‘It’s little.’
43. ƛ̕ə+√ƛ̕ə́w̕ƛ̕əʔ ‘A little bit; few.’
(ƛ̕ə́w̕ƛ̕əʔ ‘less’)
Examples 42 and 43 are similar in form and may involve
a double reduplication that is not yet understood.
2.3.2.3. When the morphemes ‘diminutive’ and ‘actual’
cooccur in a stem the ordinary realization of the ‘actual’ (see
§2.3.5) for the stem is replaced. The simple ‘actual’ has several
different morphologically and phonologically conditioned allomorphs:
reduplicative, infixed, and stress shifted/metathesized. Though the
evidence is limited, the ‘actual’ of the ‘diminutive’ seems to be formed
uniformly with C1V́+ reduplication. Compare the following:
44. √štə́ŋ ‘He walked.’
45. ʔiʔ √šə́təŋ ‘He’s walking now.’ [ACCOM
√walk(ACT)]
46. ʔiʔ šə́+√štəŋ ‘He’s taking a little walk.’
[ACCOM DIM(ACT)+√walk]
Example 44 is the simple ‘non-actual’ form; 45 is the
ordinary metathesized ‘actual’. Example 46 is the ‘actual’ of the
‘diminutive’.
Another case is the word for ‘river’ which is ordinarily in the ‘actual’
form with the infixed /(ʔ)/ (§2.3.5.2) realized as glottalization
on the /l/:
47. s√tál̕əw̕ ‘River.’
48. s-tá+√tələw̕ ‘Creek.’
Note that the /l/ in 48 is not glottalized. It is the
diminutive of example 47 and exhibits the C1V́+ form of
the ‘actual’ of the
‘diminutive’. This may not be simply a substitute
reduplicative pattern but a case of the metathesized allomorph of the ‘actual’
replacing the infixed allomorph.
See §2.3.8 on the relative ordering of the radical morphological
processes.
2.3.2.4. The C1V́+ form of the ‘diminutive’ has been
recorded also in one ‘non-actual’ form:
49. pú+√ps ‘Small cat.’ (√pus ‘cat’; √pus=áɬ
‘kitten’)
The root in example 49 happens to be borrowed, but it is
doubtful that this is the reason for
its apparently unique ‘diminutive’.3
Borrowed words in Saanich generally take the least marked allomorph for any
morpheme. Strictly morphologically conditioned variation in
derived forms of
borrowed words is, if non-existent, extremely rare. It makes sense then to
look for some phonological factors influencing the form of the ‘diminutive’ in
example 49.
This word, /pús/, also happens to be the only one syllable, CVC stem
recorded with a corresponding ‘diminutive’. The expected but non-occurring
form for the diminutive of /pús/, *pəpúʔs, would be unusual in that it
carries ultimate stress. Some forms of the ‘plural’
(§2.3.3.4), for instance, have special allomorphs that have the effect
of avoiding final word stress. For example, the ‘plural’ of the word for ‘cat’
is not *pəlús but /púʔləs/. Only one syllable stems have this allomorph
of the ‘plural’.
It seems likely then that the form of the ‘diminutive’ in example 49
is phonologically conditioned. We can expect the ‘diminutive’ of other CVC
stems to have C1V́+ reduplication.
2.3.3. Plural.
The term ‘plural’ is a bit misleading in that the
meaning of this morpheme does not exactly correspond to usual notions of
purality. This morpheme usually indicates a ‘collective’ idea, referring to a
group of items considered together rather than a number of items considered
individually. Translations usually are in the form of phrases such as "a bunch
of ..." or "lots of ...". Generally, in intransitive forms the plurality
reflects on the subject, in transitives on the object. Plurality as a category
in Saanich is obligatorily marked in only the first person subjects, objects,
and posessives (see §2.4).
There are four major allomorphs of the ‘plural’.
/(l)/ is by far the
most common and itself has some variation (§2.3.3.4). +C1í is
less common and usually phonologically predictable (§2.3.3.1). The
other two major allomorphs both involve C1C2+. One occurs fairly
frequently and is phonologically predictable (§2.3.3.3), while the
other is rare and morphologically conditioned (§2.3.3.2).
Aside from these four, there are a number of words that seem to remain
unmarked in the plural (§2.3.3.5). And, there are a few apparently
irregularly marked plurals (§2.3.3.6).
2.3.3.1. +C1í ‘plural’. About fifty examples of
this form have been recorded in quite common words. It has never been recorded
in European loans (see §2.3.3.4).
This form exhibits regressive or infixal4
reduplication of the first consonant of the root accompanied by the replacement
of the stressed /ə/ of the root by /í/5.
This form of the plural is generally predictable given the following
conditions:
A. The root is marked for plural at all.
B. The stem has no vowel but /ə/.
C. The root does not begin with /ʔ/.
D. The stem involves no other form of reduplication.
A fifth condition might be that the root not be a borrowed
form. Stems have been recorded that are exceptions: /t̕θə́x̣tən/
‘poison’, /štə́mkʷəs/
‘automobile’, /škʷəstə́nəŋ/ ‘bridge’, /šq̕ə́m̕sət/
‘trail’, /q̕x̣ʷəw̕ɬ/ ‘big canoe’, and /x̣əlx̣ə́kʷəɬ/ ‘war canoe’. Another
exception, /hə́mən/ ‘hammer’, is apparently a borrowing.
The first four are considered "modern" words. The last two are likely
loans from Wakashan.
Each of these
exceptions takes the /(l)/ form of the plural (§2.3.3.4) even
though they all have only /ə/ as vowels, do not have /ʔ/ as C1 and are
not reduplicated. /ŋə́nəʔ/ ‘offspring’ is another exception,
but it does not
take the infix. See §2.3.3.2.
Any other stem that does not meet these conditions does not
form the ‘plural’ with +C1í.
Some examples are:
50. š√qʷə+qʷíl̕s ‘Pots.’
(šqʷəl̕s ‘pot for
cooking’)
51. s√ƛ̕ə+ƛ̕íwən ‘Several
pairs of earrings.’
(ƛ̕ə́wən ‘earrings’)
52. s√wə+wíl-tən ‘Nets.’
(swə́ltən ‘net, web’)
53. s√q̕ə+q̕íməl̕ ‘Paddles.’ (sq̕ə́məl̕ ‘canoe
paddle’)
54. √k̕ʷə+k̕ʷíləw̕ ‘A bunch of hides.’
(k̕ʷə́ləw̕ ‘hide, skin’)
55. s√mə+mít=əqsən ‘Lots of snot.’ (smətəqsən
‘nasal mucus’)
56. √sə+símiʔ ‘Blankets.’ (səmə́y̕ ‘blanket’)
57. s√mə+míyəθ ‘A group of deer.’ (smə́yəθ
‘deer’)
58. √ŋə+ŋíqsən ‘A lot of noses.’ (ŋə́qsən
‘nose’)
59. √tə+tíŋəxʷ-s ‘They’re his
lands.’ (tə́ŋəxʷs
‘It’s his land.’)
60. s√nə+níxʷəɬ
‘Canoes.’ (snə́xʷəɬ ‘canoe’)
61. s√pə+píɬx̣ən
‘Valleys.’ (spə́ɬx̣ən ‘valley,
field’)
62. s√x̣ə+x̣ínəʔ ‘Legs.’
(sx̣ə́nəʔ ‘leg’)
63. √čə+čínəs ‘Teeth.’ (čə́nəs ‘tooth’)
2.3.3.2. C1C2+ ‘plural’. A very small number of stems
form the ‘plural’ with this reduplicative pattern. Only three stems involving
two roots have been observed. These stems occur frequently and have obvious
semantic similarity:
64. s-ɬən+√ɬéni=ʔ ‘Women.’ (sɬéniʔ ‘woman’)
65. s-ɬən+√ɬə̀nəč=áʔ=aɬ ‘Girls.’
(sɬənəčáʔaɬ ‘Girl’)
66. ŋən+√ŋə́n=əʔ-s ‘Her/his children.’ (ŋə́nəʔ
‘son, daughter’)
2.3.3.3. C1C2+ ‘plural’ with
C2 being either /l/
or /l̕/. About twenty examples of this ‘plural’ have been recorded. The
rule can be stated as follows: stems that do not form the
‘plural’ by +C1í
(§2.3.3.1) and have /l/ or /l̕/
in C2 position form the
‘plural’ by regressive reduplication of the first and second consonants (with
epenthetic /ə/).
Three forms have been found to be exceptions6:
/kʷəlíqən/ ‘small
bare hill’, /šməlq̕ʷéləʔ/ ‘grave’, and /x̣əlx̣ə́kʷəɬ/ ‘war canoe’. Note
that this last is also one of the exceptions to the +C1í
form of ‘plural’
mentioned in §2.3.3.1. And as with those exceptions the plural of
each of these is formed with the infix (§2.3.3.4).
Some examples:
67. s-t̕əl̕+√t̕él̕=ŋəxʷ
‘Medicines.’
(st̕él̕ŋəxʷ ‘medecine’)
68. s-ʔəl+√ʔéləxʷ ‘Elders.’
(sʔéləxʷ ‘elder’)
69. s-təl̕+√tál̕əw̕ ‘Rivers.’ (stál̕əw̕
‘river’)
70. s-ƛ̕əl+√ƛ̕éləqəm ‘Fierce animals.’
(sƛ̕éləqəm ‘fierce animal’)
71. səl+√séləs ‘Hands.’ (séləs ‘hand’)
72. s-qəl+√qéləx̣ ‘Root diggers.’ (qéləx̣ ‘root
digger’)
73. t̕əl̕+√t̕íl̕əm̕ ‘They’re singing.’
(t̕íl̕əm̕ ‘He’s singing.’)
74. s-ɬəl+√ɬáləč ‘Thighs.’ (sɬáləč ‘thigh’)
75. səl+√síləʔ ‘Grandparents.’ (síləʔ
‘grandparent’)
76. s-qəl+√qəl-él=ŋəxʷ ‘Trees.’ (sqəlélŋəxʷ
‘tree’)
2.3.3.4. /(l)/ ‘plural’. This is by far the most
common form of the ‘plural’. Over two hundred words have been recorded using
this infix.
2.3.3.4.1. This is the ‘plural’ marker for all words that
can be marked for ‘plural’ (see §2.3.3.5 for those that are not marked
and §2.3.3.6 for irregular plurals) except those that meet the
conditions for +C1í (§2.3.3.1), those that take C1C2+
(§2.3.3.2 and §2.3.3.3), and those listed in §2.3.3.6.
The placement of this infix follows simple
and quite clear-cut rules7:
/əl/ is placed immediately after the first consonant of the
root
(see examples 77 through 90)
unless 1) the word consists of only one (C)CVC syllable,
in which case /ʔlə/
is placed after the vowel, which if it is /i/ becomes /e/
(examples 91 through 96), or 2) the second
consonant of the root is /ʔ/ and it is followed by another consonant, in
which case /lə/ is placed between the first vowel and the glottal stop.
(examples 97 through 101).
The conditions for these two restrictions rarely occur. The examples
listed here (91 - 101) are, in fact, the only ones found.
77. s√n(əl)s-éʔmən ‘It’s a lot of oil.’ (Compare
with example 94 below. The suffix here is unidentified.)
78. s√ʔ(əl)áθ=əs ‘Faces.’ (sʔáθəs ‘face’)
79. √š(əl)p=ə́qs-t ‘He sharpened some points (e.g.
pencils).’ (špə́qst ‘He sharpened it.’)
80. s√ɬ(əl)p̕=áθən ‘Lips.’ (sɬp̕áθən ‘lip’)
81. √s(əl)éq-əɬ
‘They’re outside.’ (séqəɬ
‘He’s outside.’)
82. √h(əl)íʔəw̕ ‘Everybody’s sitting in the bow.’
(ʔiʔ híʔəw̕ ‘He’s sitting in the bow.’)
83. √m(əl)áay̕ ‘Baskets.’ (máay̕ ‘basket’)
84. √ŋ(əl)íq̕-ən ‘Masts.’ (ŋíq̕ən ‘mast’)
85. š√q̕(əl)ə́m̕-sət ‘Trails.’ (šq̕ə́m̕sət
‘trail, path’)
86. √t̕θ(əl)əm̕=éen ‘Arrows.’ (t̕θəm̕éen
‘arrow’. This contains the same root as example 91 below.)
87. √s(əl)éčs ‘Aunts, uncles.’ (sečs ‘aunt,
uncle’)
88. √š(əl)əmén-s ‘They’re his enemies.’ (šəméns
‘his enemy’)
89. √t̕θ(əl)ə́x̣-tən ‘A lot of poison.’ (t̕θə́x̣tən
‘poison’)
90. √kʷ(əl)əníŋət ‘They’re running.’ (kʷəníŋət
‘run’)
Bases with (C)CVC:
91. s√t̕θá(ʔlə)m̕
‘Bones.’ (√t̕θam̕. Compare
with example 86 above.)
92. s√čá(ʔlə)ɬ ‘Lots of wood.’ (√čaɬ)
93. √sá(ʔlə)ɬ ‘Roads; doors.’ (√saɬ)
94. √ná(ʔlə)s ‘They’re fat.’ (√nas)
95. √x̣é(ʔlə)č̕ sən
‘I got all scratched up.’
(x̣éč̕ sən ‘I got scratched.’)
96. √ƛ̕é(ʔlə)w̕ ‘They
ran away.’ (√ƛ̕iw̕)
Bases with ...VʔC:
97. √mé(lə)ʔkʷəɬ ‘They got hurt.’ (√méʔkʷəɬ
‘hurt’)
98. √q̕é(lə)ŋi=ʔ
‘Several maidens.’ (q̕éʔŋiʔ
‘maiden’)8
99. nə-s√čé(lə)ʔč=əʔ ‘They’re my friends.’
(nəsčéʔčəʔ ‘my friend’)
100. √ʔé(lə)ʔləŋ ‘Houses.’ (√ʔéʔləŋ)
2.3.3.4.2. When a borrowed word is marked for plural it
takes /(l)/ like native words:
101. √pú(ʔlə)s ‘Cats.’ (√pus)
102. √k(əl)əpú ‘Coats.’ (kəpú < French "capot")
103. s√t(əl)ákən ‘Socks.’ (stákən < English)
104. √k(əl)áa ‘Cars.’ (káa < English)
105. √w(əl)íič ‘Wedges.’ (wíič < English)
106. √s(əl)plíl ‘Lots of bread.’ (səplíl < Chinook Jargon)
107. √ʔ(əl)épəls ‘Apples.’ (ʔépəls < English)
108. √l(əl)klí ‘Keys.’ (ləklí < French "la clef")
2.3.3.4.3. This form of the plural has been found to occur
in words involving other types of radical morphological processes.
Reduplicated material is treated as part of the root, not as a prefix.
109. š(əl)ə+√šk̕ʷá(ʔə)m̕ ‘There’s a bunch of them
swimming.’ [ACT(PL)+√swim(ACT)]
110. k̕ʷ(əl)ə+√k̕ʷə́w̕yəkʷ ‘Little fishhooks.’
[DIM(PL)+√fishhook] (Compare
with §2.3 example 40.)
111. s-t(əl)á+√tələw̕ ‘Creeks.’
[S-DIM,ACT(PL)+√river] (Compare with §2.3 examples 47,
48, and 69.)
112. k̕ʷ(əl)ə+√k̕ʷátəʔ ‘Crows.’
[DIM(PL)+√raven]
113. q̕(əl)ə́m̕+√q̕əm̕ ‘It’s cut up (into a lot of
strips).’ [CHAR(PL)+√cut off]
(q̕ə́m̕q̕əmət sən ‘I cut it up.’; q̕ə́m̕ət sən ‘I cut it off.’)
114. s-qʷ(əl)ə́l̕+√qʷəl̕ ‘Several stories.’
[S-CHAR(PL)+√speak]
One example has been observed of this suffix being
treated as part of the root in a reduplicative process:
115. x̣(əl)+√x̣(əl)ə́ƛ̕-sət ‘It’s stormy; the wind’s
blowing and blowing.’ [REPET(PL)+√wind(PL)-REFL] (x̣éƛ̕ ‘It’s
windy; the water’s rough’; x̣éƛ̕sət ‘It’s a storm.’)
2.3.3.5. There are a number of words for which no plural
form could be elicited and all tries at plural formation were rejected. This
may involve a zero allomorph for the plural, but I suspect that for many, if
not all, of these there are pragmatic reasons for rejecting plural forms.
These reasons are sometimes easy to see. For example, there are two words for
‘raven’, /sk̕ʷtáʔ/ and /spáal̕/. The second cannot be pluralized because it
is the special word for the character ‘Raven’ in traditional stories and there
is only one ‘Raven’. In another instance the word for ‘moose’, /kʷéwəʔəč/,
cannot be pluralized because "you never see a bunch of moose,
just one or two."
In the same vein, it was difficult to elicit plurals for /sq̕ʷáŋiʔ/
‘head’
and /mén/ ‘father’ since "you can only have one." Plurals of these were
elicited, however, through the invention of appropriate contexts: "There’s a
bunch of fish heads" and "All the boys’ fathers ...".
For the words in the following list even imagined contexts were
unsuccessful in eliciting plurals: /qéq/ ‘baby’, /ŋéqəʔ/ ‘snow’,
/sθíməʔ/ ‘ice’, /sqəl̕éw̕/
‘beaver’, /sqʷáməʔ/ ‘catfish’,
/t̕θə́məkʷəʔ/ ‘perch’, /q̕ʷál̕əxʷ/ ‘chum salmon’, /ŋéʔəqs/ ‘pitch
torch’, /sməčə́ʔs/ ‘lump on a tree’, /pqʷéy̕/ ‘rotten wood’, /ɬə́q̕əs/
‘edible seaweed’, /sqéwθ/ ‘potato’, /kʷə́mkʷəm/ ‘drum’, /qʷčmín/
‘dancer’s rattle’, /č̕át̕əʔ/ ‘clam barbecuing stick’, /ləšás/ ‘angel’,
/ləmətú/ ‘sheep’, and /sŋénət/ ‘sinker for fish line’.
The last three items are particularly puzzling. The words for ‘angel’
and ‘sheep’ are obviously borrowings and many other similar loans readily take
the infixed ‘plural’. Why not these two?9
The word
for ‘sinker’, also meaning ‘rock, mountain’, is not a borrowing. One
can say /ŋə́n̕ sŋénət/ meaning ‘many stones’, ‘many mountains’, or ‘many
sinkers’, but /ŋə́n̕ sŋəlénət/ can mean only ‘many stones’ or
‘many
mountains’. /sŋénət/ as ‘sinker’ cannot be marked for plural.
2.3.3.6. Five words have been found to have plural forms
that do not fit into any of these patterns:
116. méʔən ‘Fathers.’ (mén ‘father’)
117. téʔən ‘Mothers.’ (tén ‘mother’)
118. swáʔəw̕ləs ‘Young men.’ (swíw̕ləs ‘young
man’)
119. sw̕wə́y̕qəʔ ‘Men.’ (swə́y̕qəʔ ‘man’)
120. čiyénəxʷ ‘Lots of fish.’ (sčéenəxʷ ‘fish,
salmon’)
2.3.4. /C1C2+/ ‘repetitive’. The limited number of
examples of this morpheme in the corpus is probably due to the fact that I made
no specific attempts to elicit it rather than to its natural scarcity. Only ten
examples have been recorded.
Though similar in form and meaning to the reduplicative forms of the
‘plural’, ‘repetitive’ differs in a number of respects and must be considered a
distinct morpheme. ‘Plural’ and ‘repetitive’ are not in complementary
distribution. The same root can occur with either or both morphemes with
corresponding differences in meanings. Where a predicate in the ‘plural’
refers to the number of the subject or object, a predicate marked ‘repetitive’
refers to iterations of the action or state expressed in the predicate itself.
The following examples show ‘repetitive’ in contrast to ‘actual’ and ‘plural’
of ‘actual’.
121. nəč+√nə́č-ət-əŋ ‘He laughed and laughed at
him.’ [REPET+√laugh-CTRAN-PASS]
122. √nə́č-əŋ ‘He laughed.’ [√laugh-CMDL]
123. kʷɬ nə+√nə́y̕-əŋ̕ ‘He’s laughing now.’
[REAL ACT+√laugh(ACT)-CMDL]
124. n(əl)+√nə́y̕-əŋ ‘They’re laughing.’
[ACT(PL)+√laugh(ACT)-CMDL]
See example 115 above for the cooccurrence of ‘repetitive’
and ‘plural’.
Note in the following examples that the ‘repetitive’ is sometimes
translated with English progressive ‘-ing’ and sometimes with English plural.
Nevertheless, it is distinct from the Saanich ‘actual’, usually translated
with ‘-ing’ forms in English, and ‘plural’ morphemes.
125. xʷ-s-ʔəč+√ʔə́č+əč ‘He’s stuttering.’
(See §2.3.7 on the second reduplication in this form.)
126. x̣ʷəmx̣ʷəməyásət ‘He’s spinning like a top.’
127. xʷ-ƛ̕əl+√ƛ̕əlaʔ=ás ‘He’s looking around (moving
head back and forth).’
128. nəq+√nə́q-əŋ ‘He keeps diving and coming up,
diving and coming up.’ (nə́qəŋ ‘He dove.’)
129. čəqʷ+√čə́qʷ-t-əs ‘He’s turning the light off
and on and off and on.’ (čə́qʷət ‘Light it.’)
130. nət̕θ+√nə́t̕θəʔ kʷə nə-s-iʔ √səʔéw̕-əs ‘I
put them down one by one.’ [REPET+√one SUB 1POS-S-? √put
down(ACT)]
131. θəŋ+√θéŋ ‘Twice.’ (sθəŋnéts ‘second day,
Tuesday’)
132. ʔən̕+√ʔén̕ ‘Too much.’ (√ʔen̕ ‘much’)
133. š-t̕θəm̕+√t̕θám̕=əčən ‘Several bracelets.’
(št̕θám̕əčən ‘bracelet’)
134. x̣əŋ+√x̣éŋ-ələʔ ‘Hawk.’ (Root meaning unknown)
Example 133 is translated as plural and a ‘repetitive’
reading of the form is not obvious. However, this form contains the root
/t̕θam̕/ ‘bone’ the ‘plural’ of which is /t̕θáʔləm̕/, the expected infix
‘plural’ form (see §2.3.3.4.1 examples 86 and 91).
2.3.5. Actual aspect.
The ‘actual’10
aspect is opposed to ‘nonactual’ and signals that the
action, state, or other reference of the predicate is actually occurring at an
indicated time. It is often translated into English in the form of ‘be ...
-ing’ progressive aspect and the English progressive is nearly always rendered
in the ‘actual’ in Saanich. However, a predicate in the ‘actual’ need not
refer to a continuous or progressing action. See especially examples 139-140
in this section and examples 47-48 in §2.3.2.3.
Predicates in the ‘actual’ often occur with the proclitics /kʷɬ/ ‘already’
and /ʔiʔ/ ‘accompanying’.
2.3.5.0.1. Around three hundred examples of the ‘actual’
have been recorded. It is the most frequently occurring aspect after the
‘nonactual’ and quite easy to elicit.
2.3.5.0.2. It has three primary forms: /(ʔ)/
(§2.3.5.1), C1V́+ (§2.3.5.2), and stress
shift/metathesis (§2.3.5.4) and one rather marginal form:
C1+ (§2.3.5.3). Each of these forms is accompanied by the
glottalization of all non-initial resonants. The distribution of these forms is
largely, but not entirely, phonologically predictable.
2.3.5.1. /(ʔ)/ ‘actual’. This is the most common form
of the ‘actual’. This form occurs in almost half of all recorded occurrences
of the ‘actual’ aspect.
The complete picture of the rules for the placement of this infix
involves references to rules for the placement of other morphological processes
with which /(ʔ)/ is in complementary distribution.
For the sake of
perspicuity the distribution of the most obviously regular cases of the most
common allomorph will be described first. As the distributions of the other
allomorphs are described, the rule for the placement of this infix will be
simplified.
2.3.5.1.1. A first approximation to the rule for placement
of the infix can be stated as follows:
In monosyllabic stems with roots of the shapes CV,
CVVC, or CVCC or in multi-syllabic stems where A) an underlying
/í/, /é/, or /á/ is followed by either 1) /ʔ/ or 2) one or no
consonants, or B) /ə́/ is followed by a resonant, the ‘actual’ is formed by
the insertion of /ʔ/ after the stressed vowel.
There are exceptions to this rule (§2.3.5.1.5)
which will contribute to a reformulation in §2.3.5.4.3.4.
This rule for the placement of the ‘actual’ infix includes more than it
excludes. Specifically, it excludes the environments for the C1V́+
(§2.3.5.2) and stress shift/metathesis (§2.3.5.3)
forms. So, in other words, a /ʔ/ infixed after the stressed
vowel forms the
‘actual’ unless 1) the stressed vowel of the root is /ə/ followed by an
obstruent (but see §2.3.5.1.4), 2) the vowel is followed by two
consonants the first of which is not /ʔ/, or 3) the ‘non-actual’ stem is a single
syllable, i.e. it has only one vowel. When stress of the
base is not in the root but in a suffix, the
‘actual’ is always formed by insertion of /ʔ/ after the stressed vowel
(§2.3.5.1.4).
2.3.5.1.2. The following examples illustrate environment
A.1 (§2.3.5.2). Note that a /ə/ is automatically
inserted between
the two glottal stops. Note also that this infixed /ʔ/ differs from the
‘diminutive’ infix. See §2.3.2.2.
135. √ʔəlé(ʔ)əʔ-nəxʷ sən
‘I’m hearing it now.’
(ʔəlén̕əxʷ sən ‘I heard something.’11 Note that
/ʔn/ is realized as
/n̕/. They are separated in the example to illustrate the structure.)
136. kʷɬ ʔəlx̣əw̕é(ʔ)əʔ ɬtə ‘We’re pit-lamping,
now.’ (ʔəlx̣əw̕éʔ ‘pit-lamp for crabs, etc.’)
2.3.5.1.3. The following examples illustrate environment
A.2. Note that when the infix precedes one or no consonants before a word
boundary (i.e. ______(C)#) a /ə/ is automatically inserted.
137. kʷɬ iʔ √ʔənʔé(ʔ)ə ‘He’s coming.’ (√ʔənʔé
‘come’)
138. kʷɬ √ɬəŋé(ʔ)ə-s tsə √t̕θís-ən ‘He’s
pulling the nail.’ (ɬəŋés tsə t̕θísən ‘He pulled the
nail.)
139. kʷɬ s√məlyí(ʔ)ə ‘He’s married.’
(məlyítəl sən ‘I got married.’
Note that with the ‘stative’ prefix the ‘actual’ forms
are glossed like the ‘resultive’. See §2.3.5.2 and
§2.3.6).
140. s√téŋ kʷə n̕-s√né(ʔ)ə-t tséʔə ‘What do you
call that there?’ [s√what SUB 2POS-S√name(ACT)-CTRAN-3OBJ(ø)
DEM]
(Root is CV.)
141. kʷɬ √sé(ʔ)ə-t sən ‘I’m sending him now.’
(sét sən ‘I sent him.’ Root is CV.)
142. x̣ʷáʔaŋ̕ ‘He’s weeping.’
(√x̣ʷáaŋ Root is CVVC.)
143. kʷɬ šáʔpt ‘He’s whistling now.’ (√šapt
Root is CVCC.)
144. ʔiʔ √θəŋé(ʔ)əʔ-t sən ‘I’m packing it (on
back) now.’ (√θəŋéʔ)
2.3.5.1.3.1. Examples 145-153 show the most frequently
occurring environment for /(ʔ)/ with the stressed
vowel followed by CV.
145. √ʔí(ʔ)t̕θ-əŋ̕ ‘He’s getting dressed.’
(ʔít̕θəŋ ‘He got dressed.’)
146. kʷɬ √ʔé(ʔ)č̕-ət sən ‘I’m wiping it now.’
(ʔéč̕ət ‘wipe it’)
147. kʷɬ √ʔí(ʔ)ɬən̕ sən ‘I’m eating now.’
(ʔíɬən ləʔ sən ‘I ate.’)
148. √čá(ʔ)q̕ʷ-əŋ̕ ‘He’s pouring with sweat.’
(čáq̕ʷəŋ ‘sweat’)
149. √tá(ʔ)qʷ-əŋ̕ ‘He’s coughing now.’
(táqʷəŋ ‘cough’)
150. √wé(ʔ)qəs ‘He’s yawning.’ (wéqəs ‘yawn’)
151. ʔiʔ √xʷí(ʔ)t-əŋ̕ ‘He’s jumping.’
(√xʷítəŋ ‘jump’)
152. √x̣é(ʔ)q̕-əŋ̕ sən
‘I’m scratching.’
(x̣éq̕əŋ sən ‘I scratched.’)
153. kʷɬ √t̕θí(ʔ)s-ət sən ‘I’m nailing it now.’
(t̕θísət sən ‘I nailed it.’)
2.3.5.1.3.2. When a resonant follows the stressed vowel in
the stem, the glottal stop infix merges with the resonant as usual.
A /ə/ in an immediately following syllable is deleted.
154. ʔiʔ ɬək̕ʷ=sə́n̕-t sən
‘I’m tripping him.’
(ɬək̕ʷsə́n-ət sən ‘I tripped him.’)
155. kʷɬ √t̕ə́m̕-t ɬtə ‘We’re hitting it.’
(t̕ə́m̕ət ɬtə ‘We hit it.’)
156. kʷɬ √kʷə́l̕-t sən ‘I’m pouring it.’
(kʷə́l-ət ‘pour it’)
157. √čə́n̕-t sən ‘I’m burying it now.’
(čə́nət sən ‘I buried it.’)
158. čéw̕t sən ‘I’m admiring him.’
159. √ɬə́l̕-t-əs ‘He’s splashing it.’
(ɬə́lətəs ‘He splashed it.’)
160. kʷɬ √ɬə́m̕x̣ʷ-t sən ‘I’m rubbing it.’
(ɬə́məx̣ʷt ‘rub it’)
161. ʔiʔ √qʷə́l̕-sət sən ‘I’m bailing (a canoe).’
(qʷə́ləsət ‘bail’)
162. √q̕ə́m̕-t sən ‘I’m breaking it.’
(q̕ə́m̕ət sən ‘I broke it.’)
2.3.5.1.4. The following are examples of the ‘actual’ infix
in strong suffixes.
163. xʷ√t̕θs=á(ʔ)s-t-əŋ̕ ‘He’s getting punched in
the face.’ [LOC√punch=face(ACT)-CTRAN-PASS]
164. kʷɬ iʔ √ʔəm̕=é(ʔ)q-t-əs ‘He’s delivering it.’
(ʔəméqtəs ‘He delivered it.’)
165. √ʔəɬp-é(ʔ)=səs ‘He’s slipping (doing something
with hands).’ (ʔəɬpésəs ‘He slipped.’)
166. √čx̣ʷ=é(ʔ)ɬsəʔ ‘He’s spitting.’ (čx̣ʷéɬsəʔ
‘spit’)
167. xʷ√məkʷ=θí(ʔ)-t-əs ‘She’s kissing him.’
(xʷməkʷθítəs ‘She kissed him.’)
2.3.5.1.5. A few forms are apparent exceptions to the
placement rules outlined above. These will prove to be no
exceptions at all when other facts related to the stress
shift/metathesis form of the ‘actual’ are considered
(§2.3.5.4.3.4).
The exceptions are of two kinds. A few stems that have /ə́/ in the
‘nonactual’ have this converted to /é/ when followed by the
‘actual’ infix
(examples 168-173). And there are some other forms where the
schwa preceded
by the ‘actual’ infix and a resonant does not delete (examples 171-176).
168. √x̣é(ʔ)š-əŋ̕ sən
‘I’m tripping.’
(x̣ə́šəŋ ‘He tripped.’)
169. kʷɬ √ɬé(ʔ)t̕əm̕ sən ‘I’m herring fishing.’
(ɬə́t̕əm)
170. kʷɬ √té(ʔ)š-əl-t sən ‘I’m turning it
upright now.’ (tə́šəlt sən ‘I turned it upright.’)
171. √ʔəɬtén̕-əŋ̕ ‘She’s berry picking.’
(ʔəɬtə́nəŋ ‘pick berries’)
172. √k̕ʷén̕-ət ‘I’m looking at it now.’
(k̕ʷə́nət sən ‘I looked at it.’)
173. kʷɬ iʔ √téy̕-əl̕ ‘He’s going upstream.’
(tə́yəl ‘go upstream’)
174. kʷɬ √ʔám̕ət ‘He’s sleeping.’ (√ʔámət)
175. kʷɬ iʔ √t̕θəʔ-íl̕-əŋ̕ sən ‘I’m mounting (a
horse) now.’ (t̕θəʔíləŋ sən ‘I got on.’)
176. ʔiʔ √sq-íl̕-əŋ̕ ‘I’m going out.’ (sqíləŋ
‘go/come out’)
2.3.5.2. C1V́+ ‘actual’. This reduplicative pattern
marks the ‘actual’ in around forty of the three hundred occurrences of this
aspect in the corpus.
2.3.5.2.1. When the stem is monosyllabic but not CV, CVVC,
or CVCC, or when the
stressed vowel in the root is followed by CCV
(where the first C is not /ʔ/), the ‘actual’ is formed
by regressive12
reduplication of the first consonant of the root and an immediately following
stressed vowel. The unstressed vowel then reduces to /ə/.
Some generalizations are made on this in
§2.3.5.4.3.5.
2.3.5.2.2. The following are a few of the clear cases of
the ‘actual’ in this form.
177. ʔiʔ ɬí+√ɬək̕ʷ=sən̕ sən ‘I’m tripping.’
(ɬík̕ʷsən sən ‘I tripped.’ Compare with 154 above.)
178. qé+√qən̕ ‘He’s stealing (something).’
(sqén̕ ‘It’s stolen.)
179. kʷí+√w̕ən̕-təl̕ ‘They’re fighting.’
(kʷíntəl ‘They fought.’ The /kʷ/ of the root is
an underlying resonant
which surfaces when taking the ‘actual’ glottalization.)
180. t̕θé+√t̕θəʔ ‘He’s riding a
horse.’
(√t̕θeʔ ‘on top’. Compare with 175 above.)
181. kʷɬ qʷə́+√qʷəl̕ ‘He’s saying it now.’
(√qʷəl̕. Compare with examples 253 and 262 below.)
182. kʷɬ té+√tiʔ ɬtə ‘We’re canoe racing.’
(√tey ‘canoe race’. Unstressed /ə/ is deleted before /y/. See
§1.5.6.)
183. čə́+y̕əq ‘He’s getting big.’ (√čəq ‘big’)
184. ʔiʔ ƛ̕í+√ƛ̕əw̕ ‘He’s running away.’
(√ƛ̕iw̕)
185. s-qé+√qəw̕ ‘He’s resting.
‘ (√qew)
186. kʷɬ ɬá+√ɬəp̕ sən ‘I’m eating (soup).’
(√ɬap̕)
187. s-kʷú+√kʷəl̕ ‘He’s going
to school.’ (skʷúl < English)
2.3.5.2.3. C1V́+ is also the form
of the ‘actual’ of the ‘diminutive’. See §2.3.2.3.
2.3.5.3. C1+ ‘actual’.
Nine words have been found to
form the ‘actual’ by regressive reduplication of the first consonant of the
root. Roots forming the ‘actual’ in this way have not been seen in stems
forming the ‘actual’ in other ways. It seems, then, that this form of the
‘actual’ is morphologically determined by the root.
The following examples are the only ones recorded with this form of
the
‘actual’.
188. šə+√šk̕ʷá(ʔə)m̕ sən ‘I’m swimming.’
(šk̕ʷám̕ ‘swim’)
189. kʷə+√kʷéč-əŋ̕ ‘He’s yelling.’ (kʷéčəŋ
‘yell’)
190. šə+√šíw̕əʔ ‘She’s urinating.’ (√šíwəʔ)
191. t̕ə+√t̕íl̕əm̕ sən ‘I’m singing.’ (t̕íləm
‘sing’)
192. pə+√púʔ ‘He’s defecating.’ (Recorded only in
the ‘actual’.)
193. kʷə+√kʷəx̣=θín̕-əŋ̕ ‘He’s screaming.’
(kʷəx̣θínəŋ ‘scream’)
194. t̕ə+√t̕íw̕iʔəɬ ‘She’s praying.’ (Recorded
only in ‘actual’.)
195. x̣ə+√x̣ɬé(ʔ)əm̕-ət ‘I’m watching it.’
(x̣ɬém̕ ‘watch’)
Note that some of these are marked with the regular
‘actual’ infix as well as with reduplication.
Analysis of forms with C1+ reduplication is difficult because this
also marks the ‘diminutive’ (§2.3.2) and the ‘resultive’
(§2.3.6). Some of the examples given here may actually be better
analyzed as containing one of these two other morphemes.
2.3.5.4. The stress shift/metathesis form of the
‘actual’ has undergone two rather different analyses for two other Straits
languages, Klallam (Thompson and Thompson, 1969) and Lummi (Demers, 1974). The
Saanich data differ significantly from both Klallam and Lummi, so, it will be
necessary to present a third analysis (§2.3.5.4.3). It will first be
useful to describe the differences between Saanich and Klallam on the one hand
(§2.3.5.4.1) and Saanich and Lummi on the other
(§2.3.5.4.2).
2.3.5.4.1. Thompson and Thompson (1969) recognize a class
of stems in Klallam that form the ‘actual’ directly by a metathesis of the
stressed vowel and the following consonant. They give a number of examples
like: čkʷút ‘shoot’, čúkʷt ‘shooting’; x̣č̕ít ‘scratch’, x̣íč̕t
‘scratching’ (p.216). Rather than assume a never surfacing root shape of CVCV
with identical vowels and a complication of Klallam’s stress rules, Thompson
and Thompson convincingly posit a morphologically conditioned metathesis for
the ‘actual’ of a certain class of stems.
Saanich differs from Klallam in that there are no pairs of forms like
those above with the position of a non-schwa in a biconsonantal root carrying
the difference between
‘actual’ and ‘nonactual’. All cases of apparent metathesized one-syllable
‘actuals’ in Saanich have only /ə́/ (see examples 198-211).
Metathesis
of non-schwas and following consonants does clearly occur in Saanich but as an
entirely phonologically conditioned process having nothing necessarily to do
with the ‘actual’. Compare example 145 above with 196 and 197:
196. ʔət̕θíŋəstxʷ ‘Get him dressed.’
197. ʔət̕θəŋístəs ‘She’s getting him dressed.’
The vowel of the strong root, √ʔít̕θ, is carried
rightward
into the syllables of weak suffixes by the maintenance of penultimate
stress.13
See §1.4 for more on Saanich stress and metathesis.
Since there is a great deal of phonological metathesis in Saanich and no
morphologically functioning metathesis of full vowels in roots with two
consonants, an analysis for the
Saanich ‘actual’ different from Thompson and Thompson’s analysis of Klallam
seems advisable.
2.3.5.4.2. The data presented by Demers (1974) for Lummi
match Saanich more closely than Klallam does. All examples of apparent
‘actual’ metathesis listed by Demers involve /ə́/. Demers, assuming for
Lummi the alternative analysis suggested by Thmms as involving
underlying Cə́Cə roots. A stress protraction rule
moves the stress across a single obstruent onto the second syllable when it is
followed by two consonants. This then accounts for Lummi ‘nonactuals’ like t̕sə́tsən ‘I smashed it’, t̕ə́s ‘it got smashed’. The ‘actuals’
like
t̕ə́st ‘he’s breaking it’ are then accounted for as involving the regular
glottal stop infix for ‘actual’, which evidently works in
Lummi similarly to the
way it works in Saanich (§2.3.5.1). Thus the underlying form of Lummi
t̕ə́st is ∥t̕ə́ʔsət∥. The unstressed
schwa is then deleted and all glottal
stops between schwas and obstruents are deleted. This is an elegant solution
for Lummi. It is, however, not viable for Saanich for two reasons.
First, the Lummi stress protraction rule has no Saanich analog. A
number of forms have /ə́/ between the second root consonant and only one
following consonant14 (see examples 198-211)
and in the second syllable when the first is followed by two consonants (see
examples 213-215 for a few of the many occurrences of this).
And, conversely, there are a number of forms that have /ə́/
in the first
syllable when the second syllable is followed by two consonants (e.g.
/tə́šəlt sən/ ‘I turned it upright’; /q̕ə́ləp̕sət/ ‘It shrank’;
/q̕ʷə́lət̕θt sən/ ‘I peeled it’).
Second, Saanich /ʔ/ does not ordinarily delete between /ə/ and an
obstruent as it does in Lummi (e.g. /ŋə́nəʔs/ ‘his/her child’;
/t̕ét̕əʔsət sən/ ‘I’m trying to learn it’; /sčəčə́ʔsəqʷ/ ‘little hat’).
These two facts demand a different approach to Saanich.
2.3.5.4.3. The following are examples of ‘actual’ and
‘nonactual’ pairs. In each of these pairs the (a) member is the ‘nonactual’
and (b) is the ‘actual’.
198a. t̕θq̕ə́ŋ ‘It dripped.’
b. t̕θə́q̕əŋ̕ ‘It’s dripping.’
199a. č̕tə́ŋ ‘He crawled.’
b. č̕ə́təŋ̕ ‘He’s crawling.’
200a. k̕ʷsə́ŋ ‘He counted.’
b. k̕ʷə́səŋ̕ ‘He’s counting.’
201a. ƛ̕k̕ʷə́t ‘Put it out (a fire).’
b. ƛ̕ə́k̕ʷt sən ‘I’m putting it out.’
202a. xʷq̕p̕ət ‘Patch it.’ (/xʷ-/ ‘locative’)
b. xʷq̕ə́p̕t sən ‘I’m patching it.’
203a. sq̕ə́t sən ‘I tore it (intentionally).’
b. kʷɬ sə́q̕təs ‘He’s tearing it.’
204a. sx̣ə́t ‘Push it.’
b. kʷɬ sə́x̣təs ‘He’s pushing it.’
205a. šč̕ə́təs ‘He whipped it.’
b. ʔiʔ šə́č̕təs ‘He’s whipping it.’
206a. štə́ŋ ‘He walked.’
b. kʷɬ šə́təŋ̕ ‘He’s walking.’
207a. tkʷə́t ‘Break it (a stick).’
b. tə́kʷtəs ‘He’s breaking it.’
208a. tqʷə́t ‘Tighten it.’
b. kʷɬ tə́qʷt sən ‘I’m tightening it.’
209a. t̕sə́t sən ‘I broke it (intentionally)’
b. t̕ə́st sən ‘I’m breaking it.’
210a. θk̕ʷə́t ‘Straighten it out.’
b. θə́k̕ʷt sən ‘I’m straightening it out.’
211a. θx̣ə́t ‘Shove him.’
b. kʷɬ θə́x̣t sən ‘I’m shoving it.’
2.3.5.4.3.1. Since in all other forms of this opposition
the ‘actual’ is the marked form and stems in the ‘nonactual’ constitute the
base forms, we may assume that the ‘nonactuals’ of these pairs exhibit the base
forms. These bases have a number of things in common. Two things are obvious:
1) they each have /ə́/ following the two consonants of the
root and
preceding the single consonant of the suffix, and 2) they are all
monosyllabic.
Given this second fact it might be expected that these bases would form
the ‘actual’ the way all other monosyllabic bases involving roots with two
consonants and one vowel do, that is, with C1V́+ reduplication (see
§2.3.5.2). That they do not might suggest that these are (as in
Lummi) underlying CVCV bases. But, as noted above
(§2.3.5.4.2), the then necessary stress protraction and glottal stop
deletion rules are otherwise unmotivated in Saanich.
Another less abstract possibility, and the one for which there seems to
be support, is that these bases do not form the actual by C1V́+
reduplication because there simply is no underlying root vowel. In each of
these bases the underlying root is CC, vowelless, and therefore the stress falls
on
the suffix. The analysis of 209a, for example, would be as 212.
212. √t̕s-ə́t [√break-CTRAN]
2.3.5.4.3.3. In each of these bases the root occurs with
either ∥-əŋ∥ ‘middle’ or ∥-ət∥ ‘control transitive’. It happens that
these are the only roots that allow stress to fall on these suffixes. Support
for this analysis comes from the fact that there are a number of other suffixes
whose underlying vowel surfaces only when following these roots.
The ‘non-control transitive’ suffix ∥-naxʷ∥
is an example.15
The following three examples contain the roots of examples 203, 207, and
209. The schwa within the root is predictable (see §1.5.10).
213. √səq̕-náxʷ sən ‘I tore it accidentally; I
finally managed to tear it.’
214. √təkʷ-náxʷ sən ‘I broke it accidentally; I
finally managed to break it.’
215. √t̕əs-náxʷ sən ‘I broke it accidentally; I
finally managed to break it.’
2.3.5.4.3.4. Recognizing this set of roots as underlyingly
vowelless allows us a non-arbitrary way of dealing with some of the exceptions
to the infixed form of the ‘actual’ mentioned in §2.3.5.1.5 and,
further, to generalize the rule for ‘actual’ infix placement
(§2.3.5.1.1).
Part A of this rule states that the glottal stop infix
follows the stressed vowel unless it is /ə/. Some of the forms listed in
§2.3.5.1.5 were exceptions to this (part B and the other exceptions
are dealt with in §2.3.5.4.3.6). The proviso against the infix
following /ə́/ was made precisely because of the existence of
the set of
roots
exemplified in 198-211. Now that these roots are out of consideration
as having /ə/ in the underlying form, part A of the rule can
be restated as
follows:
In monosyllabic stems with roots of the shapes CV,
CVVC, or CVCC or in multi-syllabic stems where an underlying stressed vowel is
followed by either /ʔ/ or one or no consonants, /ʔ/ is inserted after the
stressed vowel and /ə́/ is replaced by /é/.
The underlying distinction is between roots with no vowels
(examples 198-211) and roots with underlying /ə́/ (examples 168-173 in
§2.3.5.1.5). More facts that support the analysis of this underlying
distinction are discussed in regard to the resultive in §2.3.6.3.
See also §2.5.2.3, §2.5.4.5, and §2.5.4.7 for other suffixes whose
underlying vowels surface only with underlyingly vowelless roots.
2.3.5.4.3.5. Now that the underlying vowelless nature of
the roots has been established the problem remains how to account for the
‘actual’ forms in examples 198-211. Though the precise nature of the
mechanism is as yet unclear16
I would like to suggest that the ‘actual’ forms in these examples are
functionally allied with the C1V́+ reduplicative form (§2.3.5.2).
Both the C1V́+ reduplicated and metathesized forms of the ‘actual’ are
primarily characterized by a leftward shift in stress. Then, as with the other
stress placement rules (mentioned above in §2.3.5.4.1 examples
196-197 and in §1.4), the stressed vowel of the base moves to
accommodate the position. When
stress is shifted leftward beyond the first
consonant of the root, a copy of that consonant is carried along.
This stress shift in both cases is triggered by phonological
environments complementary to those for the ‘actual’ infix placement described
in §2.3.5.1 and revised in §2.3.5.4.3.4. The ‘actual’ is
marked by stress shift (with concomitant reduplication or metathesis) either
when the base is monosyllabic or when the stressed vowel is in the initial
syllable and followed by CCV. When roots taking C1V́+ ‘
actual’ in
monosyllabic stems are involved in polysyllabic forms, the ‘actual’ is formed
with the glottal stop infix (see examples 177 and 180 in
§2.3.5.2.2). Similarly, when roots taking metathesized ‘actuals’
(examples 198-211) in monosyllabic stems are involved in polysyllabic
forms, the ‘actual’ is formed with the glottal stop infix. Compare 216 to
206.
216a. štəŋístəs ‘He walked/ran/operated it.’
b. kʷɬ štəŋ̕íʔstəs ‘He’s operating it (a
machine).’
2.3.5.4.3.6. Part B of the ‘actual’ infix placement rule
(§2.3.5.1.1) states that the glottal stop is infixed after a /ə́/
with no change in vowel quality (see revision of part A of the rule in
§2.3.5.4.3.4) only when the consonant following the stress is a
resonant. Given the analysis of the previous two sections and in particular
the revised version of part A of the infix placement rule
we can see that part B is unnecessary. The infix occurs after underlying /ə́/
before obstruents as well.
Each of the metathesized ‘actuals’ in examples 198-211 involves a
vowelless root with an obstruent as the second consonant. The question that
now arises is whether or not there are vowelless roots with a resonant
in C2
position and, if there are, how they form the actual.
The vowelless roots with obstruents in C2 position allow
stress to
fall on the transitive suffixes ∥-ət∥ and ∥-naxʷ∥.
There are roots
with a resonant as the second consonant that allow stress to fall on the
transitive suffix:
217. √t̕əm̕-náxʷ sən ‘I hit it accidentally; I
finally managed to hit it.’
218. √q̕əm̕-náxʷ sən ‘I cut it (in two)
accidentally; I finally managed to cut it.’
The schwa within the root is predictable as in examples
213-215 above.
In §2.3.5.4.3.4 and §2.3.5.4.3.5 it was established that
there are two types of ‘nonactual’ bases with obstruents in C2 showing /ə/
on the surface: 1) those with underlying /ə/ which form the
‘actual’ by a
glottal stop infix and the change of /ə́/ to /é/, and 2) those with no
underlying vowel which form the ‘actual’ by stress shift/metathesis.
A
parallel distinction is found in ‘nonactual’ bases with surface /ə/ and
a resonant in C2 position: 1) those with underlying /ə/ form the ‘actual’
just as do bases with an obstruent as the second root consonant, with a glottal
stop infix and change of /ə́/ to /é/ (example 219 and examples 171-172
in §2.3.5.1.5), and 2) those with no
underlying vowel, which form the ‘actual’ as in examples 220-225.
219a. √kʷə́n-ət sən ‘I took it.’
b. √kʷén̕-ət sən ‘I’m taking it.’
220a. √t̕ə́m̕-ət sən ‘I hit it.’
b. √t̕əm̕-t sən ‘I’m hitting it.’
221a. √q̕ə́m̕-ət sən ‘I cut it in two.’
b. √q̕ə́m̕-t sən ‘I’m cutting it.’
222a. √k̕ʷə́n-ət sən ‘I looked at it.’
b. √k̕ʷə́n̕-t sən ‘I’m looking at it.’17
223a. √čə́n-ət ‘Bury it.’
b. √čə́n̕-t sən ‘I’m burying it.’
224a. √ɬə́l-ət sən ‘I splashed it.’
b. √ɬə́l̕-t-əs ‘He’s splashing it.’
225a. kʷə́l̕-ət ‘Pour it out.’
b. kʷɬ √kʷə́l̕-t sən ‘I’m pouring it out now.’
If we recognize that there are no root initial
obstruent-resonant sequences in Saanich, it can be seen that the ‘actual’ in
examples 220-225 are formed by the same rule as the ‘actuals’ in examples
198-211. Two otherwise well justified rules, a rule to insert a schwa into
a root initial obstruent-resonant cluster and the regular penultimate stress
assignment rule, must be ordered with respect to one
another and after the ‘actual’ stress shift/metathesis rule18.
2.3.5.4.3.7. Some sample derivations should suffice to
illustrate how these rules work. The rules are numbered 1) ‘actual’ stress
shift/metathesis, 2) schwa insertion, 3) penultimate stress assignment.
A hyphen indicates that the environment for the application of the rule is not
present.
| Actual | Nonactual | Actual | Nonactual |
U.F. | ∥t̕s-ə́t∥ | ∥t̕s-ə́t∥ | ∥t̕m̕-ə́t∥ | ∥t̕m̕-ə́t∥
1. | t̕ə́st | - | t̕ə́m̕t | -
2. | - | - | - | t̕əm̕ə́t
3. | - | - | - | t̕ə́m̕ət |
S.F. | t̕ə́st | t̕sə́t | t̕ə́m̕t | t̕ə́m̕ət |
Two more derivations show how the other apparent exceptions mentioned
in §2.3.5.1.5 are accounted for. The rule that applies inserts the
actual infix and replaces /ə́/ by /é/.
| Actual | Nonactual | Actual | Nonactual |
U.F. | ∥x̣ə́š-əŋ∥ | ∥x̣ə́š-əŋ∥ | ∥kʷə́n-ət∥ | ∥kʷə́n-ət∥ |
| x̣éʔšəŋ̕ | - | kʷéʔnət | - |
S.S. | x̣éʔšəŋ̕ | x̣ə́šəŋ | kʷén̕ət | kʷə́nət |
2.3.5.4.3.8. Thus far only roots with two
consonants have been considered. There are a few roots that have three
consonants. They form the ‘actual’ just like roots with two consonants. When
there is only one underlying vowel and it is between the second and third
consonants, the ‘actual’ is formed by stress shift/metathesis. The behavior
of these triconsonantal roots further supports the preceding analysis,
especially in cases
where the root vowel is a non-schwa.
Following are examples of triconsonantal
roots exhibiting stress shift/metathesis in the ‘actual’.
226a. √t̕θɬə́k̕ʷ-t sən ‘I pinched it.’
b. √t̕θə́ɬk̕ʷ-t sən ‘I’m pinching it.’
227a. √ləméʔ-t-əŋ sən ‘Someone kicked me.’
b. √lém̕əʔ-t-əŋ̕ sən ‘Someone’s kicking me.’
228a. √ɬəy̕ə́qʷ-t ‘Smash it up.’
b. √ɬə́y̕qʷ-t-əs ‘He’s smashing it up.’
229a. √ɬq̕élč̕ ‘Moon.’
b. √ɬéq̕əl̕č̕ ‘It’s a bright moon.’
230a. √ƛ̕əčə́q̕-t ‘Press down on it.’
b. √ƛ̕ə́y̕q̕-t-əs ‘He’s pressing down on it.’
(Note that the /č/ is underlying /y/ and that /ə/ insertion must precede
y ∼ č §1.5.9.)
231a. ƛ̕pə́x̣-t ‘Scatter it.’
b. ƛ̕ə́px̣-t ‘I’m scattering it.’
232a. √mələ́č̕-t ‘Roll over.’
b. √mə́l̕č̕-t sən ‘I’m rolling over.’
233a. √θɬə́qʷ-t ‘Pierce it.’
b. √θə́ɬqʷ-t sən ‘I’m piercing it.’
2.3.5.5. Three morphemes have special forms in the
‘actual’ : ‘diminutive’, ‘resultive’, and the ‘structured activity’ suffix.
See §2.3.2.3 on the ‘actual’ of the ‘diminutive’,
§2.3.6.3 on the ‘actual’ of the ‘resultive’, and §2.5.4.1 on
the ‘actual’ of ‘structured activity’.
2.3.5.6. The following words all seem to take a form of
the glottal stop infix, but not as expected. With each exception the expected
but non-occurring form is preceded by an asterisk.
234. čəʔáw̕əs sən ‘I’m using it now.’
(čákʷəs ‘use something’. *čáw̕əs)
235. səʔéw̕əs sən ‘I’m putting it down.’
(sékʷəs ‘put something down’. *séw̕əs)
236. ʔəl̕ál̕əɬ sən ‘I’m boarding (a canoe) now.’
(ʔáaɬ ‘go aboard’. *ʔáʔaɬ)
237. kʷɬ p̕áhaŋ̕ ‘It’s swelling up; it’s swollen.’
(p̕áaŋ̕ ‘swell up’. This may also be an exceptional form of the
‘resultive’.)
2.3.6. Resultive.
The ‘resultive’ aspect is opposed to ‘nonresultive’ and indicates an
emphasis on the result or outcome of the action expressed in the predicate.
The ‘resultive’ is usually translated into English as a participle. In
Saanich it often occurs with the ‘stative’ prefix (§2.1.2), the
‘durative’ suffix (§2.2.2), or both.
Only about sixty different examples of the
‘resultive’ appear in the corpus. Its rarity relative to the ‘actual’ aspect
is perhaps due to my belated realization of the productiveness of this
morpheme.
The ‘resultive’ has two primary forms, C1+ reduplication
(§2.3.6.1) and ablaut (§2.3.6.2).
2.3.6.1. C1+ ‘resultive’. This form appears in fewer
than half of the recorded examples of the ‘resultive’.
The C1+ form is less
often recorded with the ‘stative’ or ‘durative’ affixes than the ablaut
forms.
Bases all have /i/, /e/, or /a/.
Since this reduplicative pattern is also employed in the ‘actual’ and
since English translations can be ambiguous, it is often difficult to decide
which morpheme is represented in the form. Often both are. This problem is
particularly evident in the first two examples below.
238. t̕θə+t̕θəʔ=wéč sən ‘I’m sitting.’
[RES+√upon(√t̕θeʔ)=bottom 1SUBJ]
239. θə+/θíɬ-əŋ sən ‘I’m standing.’
240. xʷə+√xʷə́y̕əm̕ ʔə tsə √ʔéw̕k̕ʷ-s ‘She’s a
prostitute.’ [RES+√sell(ACT) OBL DEM √self-3POS]
241. kʷɬ sə+/séw̕-t ‘He’s lying down.’
[REAL RES+√put down(ACT)-STAT]
242. s-θə+√θím̕əʔ ‘It’s frozen.’ (θíməʔt
‘freeze it’)
243. s-kʷə+√kʷél̕ ‘He’s in hiding.’
[STAT-RES+√hide] (Compare with the ‘actual’
kʷé+kʷəl̕ ‘He’s hiding
now.’)
244. t̕ə+√t̕éy̕əq̕ ‘He’s angry.’
2.3.6.2. /a/ or /e/ substitute for root /ə/
‘resultive’. This is the most common form of the ‘resultive’ and the most
easily recognized. It appears both in roots with underlying /ə/ and in
underlying vowelless roots. For a particular root either /a/ or /e/
appears consistently and no phonological conditioning factors have been
discovered19. It appears, then, that two morphological classes must be
recognized for roots with surface /ə/ in the ‘nonresultive’:
those that have
/a/ in the ‘resultive’ (examples 245-252) and those that have /e/
(examples 253-261).
Forms with ‘resultive’ /a/:
245. kʷɬ txʷ-əs√k̕ʷás-əɬ ‘It’s already counted.’
[REAL MUT-STAT√count(√k̕ʷs)-DUR]
246. kʷɬ s√ƛ̕áq̕ʷ-əɬ ‘It’s stuck on.’
[REAL STAT√stick on(√ƛ̕q̕ʷ)-DUR]
247. s√mákʷ-əɬ ‘He’s curled up.’
248. √náqʷ-əɬ ləʔ sən
‘I was asleep.’
(nə́qʷ sxʷ ‘Go to sleep.’)
249. s√p̕ákʷ-əɬ ‘He’s floating.’ (√p̕əkʷ ‘rise
to the surface.’)
250. s√p̕áɬ-əɬ ‘He’s sober.’ (p̕ə́ɬ sən ‘I
sobered up.’)
251. s√táqʷ-əɬ ‘It’s tight.’ (Compare with
example 208 above.)
252. s√θák̕ʷ-əɬ ‘It’s straight.’ (Compare with
example 210 above.)
Forms with ‘resultive’ /e/:
253. s√qʷél̕ ‘Word; a speech.’ (√qʷəl̕ ‘say’.
Compare with example 181 §2.3.5.2.2.)
254. s√lét̕θ-əɬ ‘It’s full.’ (√lət̕θ ‘fill’)
255. s√q̕ép-əɬ ‘They’re together.’ (√q̕p
‘gather’)
256. s√q̕éθ-əɬ ‘It’s not enough.’
257. √séq̕-əɬ ‘It’s outside.’ (√səq̕ ‘out’)
258. s√téx̣-əɬ sxʷ
‘You’re wrong.’
259. txʷə-s√x̣éɬ-əɬ ‘He got sick.’ (x̣ə́ɬ sən ‘I feel bad.’)
260. s√léq̕-əɬ ‘It’s level.’
261. √qés-əɬ ‘He’s overboard.’ (√qəs ‘fall
overboard’)
2.3.6.3. When the ‘resultive’ occurs with the ‘actual’ the
form sometimes exhibits C1+ reduplication. Though the data
are limited, it
seems that this is the form that occurs when there is an underlying root vowel.
262. qʷə+qʷél̕ ‘He’s giving a speech.’ (Compare
with examples 181 and 253.)
263. p̕ə+√p̕é(ʔ)kʷ-əŋ̕ ‘He’s floating now.’
(Compare with example 249.)
The ‘actual’ morpheme follows normally the ‘resultive’
when a vowelless root is involved. Compare the following:
264a. √ɬq̕ʷ-ə́t-əs ‘He took it off.’ (nonactual)
b. √ɬə́q̕ʷ-t-əs ‘He’s taking it off.’ (actual)
c. √ɬáq̕ʷ-əŋ ‘He got undressed.’ (resultive)
d. √ɬá(ʔ)q̕ʷ-əŋ̕ ‘He’s getting undressed.’
(resultive and infixed form of actual)
e. s-ɬá+√ɬəq̕ʷ ‘He’s naked.’ (resultive and
reduplicative form of the actual)
2.3.7. +C2 ?. Only four examples of this reduplicative
pattern have been recorded. Although there are
not enough samples to determine a
meaning, the glosses of the recorded forms do seem to suggest that this may be
the Saanich cognate of a +C2 reduplicative morpheme that is
widespread in the
Interior Salishan languages. Carlson and Thompson (1981) discuss a +C2
reduplicative morpheme they call ‘out-of-control’. They give many examples of
this morpheme, which seems to be a sort of ‘non-control resultive’, from
Interior Salishan languages and a few from some Coast Salishan languages. At
least the first three examples here seem to have glosses that fit the
‘out-of-control’ idea.
265. ʔəw̕ √qə́l+əl ʔal̕ sən nə-š-xʷ√qʷél̕=kʷən
‘I got discouraged.’ [CONTEMP √bad-’out-of-control’? LIMIT 1SUBJ
1POS-S,LOC√say(RES)=mind]
266. xʷ-s√ʔə́č+əč ‘He stutters.’
267. √mə́l+əl ‘It got soft (butter).’
268. √wə́q̕+əq̕ ‘Tree toad.’
2.3.8. Ordering of the radical morphological processes.
By inspecting forms in which pairs of radical morphological processes
cooccur, a definite order of application can be discovered. The order described
here is not intended to be definitive but merely suggestive. Although the
order given here is completely consistent with the facts, it is based on an
assumption that is not formally founded: that the order of the radical
morphological processes is determined by the morpheme. That is, I offer no
justification for the assumption that, for example, the reduplicative ‘actual’
appears at the same point in the derivation as the infixed ‘actual’.
2.3.8.1. The following have been observed in instances of
cooccurrence of radical morphological processes:
1. Examples 3 and 4 in §2.3.1 demonstrate
that ‘characteristic’ precedes the ‘resultive’.
2. Example 264 in §2.3.6.3 shows that
‘actual’ follows ‘resultive’.
3. Examples 110-112 in §2.3.3.4.3 show
that ‘plural’ follows ‘diminutive’.
4. Though example 48 in §2.3.2.3 is
inconclusive, example 46 suggests that ‘diminutive’ precedes ‘actual’ if we
assume that the /ə́/ is the metathesized vowel of the suffix.
5. In §2.3.3.4.3 examples 109 and 111 it
can be seen that ‘actual’ must precede ‘plural’.
6. Examples 113-114 in §2.3.3.4.3 show
that ‘plural’ follows ‘characteristic’.
7. Example 115 in §2.3.3.4.3 shows that
‘plural’ precedes ‘repetitive’.
2.3.8.2. Given the facts of §2.3.8.1 the
productive radical morphological processes can be seen to apply in the
following order:
1. characteristic
2. resultive
3. diminutive (1, 2)?
4. actual
5. plural
6. repetitive
The arcs indicate pairs that have an order established
in §2.3.8.1. The relationships
between ‘diminutive’ and ‘resultive’ and between ‘diminutive’ and
‘characteristic’ have not been determined. No contradictions to this order
have been observed.
2.3.9. Unanalyzable reduplication.
A number of forms have been recorded that apparently exhibit
reduplication, but whose roots do not occur elsewhere.
269. pəpət̕θíŋ ‘Skunk.’
270. sqəqəwéθ ‘Rabbit.’
271. qə́qtəm̕as ‘Lacrosse.’
272. tíntən ‘Ring a bell.’
273. št̕θét̕θəxʷnəč ‘Tailbone.’
274. šíʔšəč̕ ‘The bushes (if nobody’s in them).’
275. šíl̕šəl̕əč̕ ‘The bushes (if somebody’s in
them).’
For the first three examples, 269-271, I have no
plausible analysis. Example 272 is probably a loan from Chinook Jargon, but
it could very well be a ‘characteristic’
(§2.3.1) form. If it is, then
the ‘actual’ form of the word,
/títən̕tən̕/ ‘ringing a bell’, establishes an ordering relatio
nship between
‘characteristic’ and ‘actual’ and supports the order described in
§2.3.8.2. Example 273 looks like, and could be, the C1
V́+ form of
the ‘actual’. An analysis of 273 would then be 276:
276. š-t̕θé+√t̕θəxʷ=n̕əč [S,LOC-ACT+√?=tail]
Examples 274 and 275 are the most interesting. They
seem to fit none of the analyzable patterns, and the glosses, confirmed by two
informants, are puzzling. The two forms must certainly be related, but how?
And these are probably related to another form, /šə́ləč̕/ ‘world, universe’,
that does not help clarify the issue.
Notes to §2.3.
1. This gloss was suggested by L. C. Thompson (p.c.).
2. The precedence can be thought of either in terms of linear order as
prefixes or as transformational processes. See §2.3.8 on the relative
ordering of the radical morphological processes.
3. It is doubtful but not entirely out of the question.
Although the word for ‘cat’ in Songish
recorded by Raffo (1972:162) is quite different from the
Saanich word, it is also borrowed
and seems to have a unique reduplicative ‘diminutive’:
/píš/ ‘cat’, /píšpiš/ ‘kitten’.
4. There is, at present, no evidence
to decide between the analysis presented here and an
analysis of this as a discontinuous morpheme with regressive,
C1+, reduplication and concomitant change of the root vowel
to /i/.
5. Raffo (1972:137) has analysed cognate forms in Songish as involving
two morphemes, a reduplicative ‘plural’ and an /-i-/ infix
‘collective’. The ‘plural’ always occurs when the
‘collective’ occurs but not vice versa.
I find no evidence in Saanich for such an analysis.
6. Kinkade (p.c.) suggests that the two /C1C2+/
forms of ‘plural’ can be viewed as the same with the general
conditioning environment C1V́RX, where V́ is not ə́ and X is not
a word boundary. This way the three exceptions here are not
exceptions at all, but example 76 then is.
This analysis is probably the correct one,
but the large number of examples with /l/ or /l̕/ as the resonant
is striking especially since these are not the most frequently
occurring resonants in the language.
7. The placement of the Saanich l-infix plural is similar to, but
simpler than, that of the Cowichan cognate described by Hukari (1982).
For Saanich too, the placement of this infix cries for a unified
explanation in terms of its
interaction with the other radical morphological processes.
Here I simply describe its distribution.
8. The /ʔ/ is lost in the ‘plural’ here. I have no
explanation for this.
9. The absence of a plural marking for /ləmətú/ may
have something to do with the fact that ‘sheep’ in English has a zero plural.
The word for ‘angel’ may represent a borrowing of a French plural, "les anges",
and so is not needed to be pluralized again.
But see example 107 above where ‘apples’, a plural form
borrowed into Saanich from English can be further pluralized.
10. This term was first used by Thompson and Thompson (1969, 1971).
11. In this and most of the following translations of the ‘nonactual’ I
use the past tense. It should be kept in mind that the Saanich
sentences are unmarked for tense except when one of
the post-predicate particles /ləʔ/ or /səʔ/ (§2.6.2.3.1) is present.
12. An alternative would be to analyze this as a progressive
reduplication. If the suggestions made in §2.3.5.4.3.5 below
are correct, the analysis as regressive reduplication is clearly preferable. An
other possibility is that C1í is infixed after the root /ə/.
13. These two forms involve the ‘relational’
suffix ∥-ŋiy∥ (§2.5.3.2) as well as the ‘causative’ (§2.5.2.3.).